Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Reserve-building activities in multiple sclerosis patients and healthy controls: a descriptive study

Abstract

Background

Cognitive reserve has been implicated as a possible protective factor in multiple sclerosis (MS) but to date no study has compared reserve-building activities across disease course or to healthy controls. This study aims to describe differences in reserve-building activities across the MS disease course and healthy controls.

Methods

Secondary analysis of a cross-sectional cohort study that included 276 healthy controls, and subjects with clinically isolated syndrome (CIS; n = 67), relapsing-remitting MS (RRMS; n = 358) and secondary progressive MS (PMS; n = 109). Past reserve-building activities were operationalized as occupational attainment and education. Current activities comprised 6 strenuous and 6 non-strenuous activities, including 5 reserve-building activities and television-watching. Multivariate Analysis of Variance models examined group differences in past and current activities, after adjusting for covariates.

Results

There were group differences in past and current reserve-building activities. SPMS patients had lower past reserve-building activities than healthy controls. All forms of MS engaged in fewer strenuous current reserve-building pursuits than healthy controls. RRMS read less than healthy controls. SPMS engaged in fewer job-related non-strenuous activities. All MS groups watched more television than healthy controls.

Conclusions

MS patients show significantly fewer past and present reserve-building activities. Although it is difficult to establish causality without future prospective studies, lifestyle-modifying interventions should prioritize expanding MS patients’ repertoire of strenuous and non-strenuous activities.

Peer Review reports

Background

The concept of resilience has been the focus of study via diverse social scientific disciplines, including behavioral medicine [1], health psychology [2], epidemiology [3], and education research [2]. Recent clinical research in neurology has revealed that cognitive reserve – a property of the nervous system enhanced by past and current salutogenic stimulating activities -- is associated with better cognitive functioning in the face of neurologic illness or injury [4]. Recent work has documented that past and current stimulating activities may be protective against progression in a broad range of disability domains in multiple sclerosis (MS) [5]. The multi-dimensionality of the factors documented to contribute to resilience is notable, going beyond cognitive activities or outcomes and extending into physical, creative, intellectual, spiritual, and cultural enrichment. Consequently, we believe the nomenclature should be changed to broaden the implied dimensionality of reserve by referring to the concept of reserve rather than “cognitive reserve”.

Reserve is conceptualized as arising from inborn, past, and current resources, and has been operationalized by measurable indicators. Inborn reserve or “brain reserve” has been operationalized as intracranial volume [6], head circumference [6], measured intelligence quotient in early life, and genetic/environmental modifiers [7]. Past reserve-building activities derive from past enrichment and achievement, and have been measured as educational and occupational attainment as well as childhood exposure to stimulating cultural and educational pursuits [8, 9]. Current reserve-building activities refer to current enrichment pursuits, and have been measured as current cultural, intellectual, physical, and spiritual leisure activities [4, 9]. These reserve-building pursuits may require new learning, leading to the development of more dendrites, dendritic spines, synapses, and perhaps even cells, all of which contribute to reserve. In particular, diverse current reserve-building pursuits may be important to maintain reserve by ensuring that more areas of the brain and interconnections remain active and fit. The concept of reserve provides a parsimonious and inclusive framework for examining how an individual can enhance health and well-being by current pursuits that build on childhood experiences and innate capacity [10].

The growing evidence base supporting the relevance and importance of reserve has generally focused on its impact in people dealing with neurological illness or injury, including MS [11], brain injury [12], Parkinson’s disease [13], Alzheimer’s disease [14], cancer chemotherapy [15], and lead exposure [16]. To our knowledge, no work has been done examining multidimensional indicators of reserve in healthy individuals and comparing them to people with an illness. Although it is common practice to compare patients to healthy controls on the basis of cognitive or neuropsychiatric symptoms in studies of MS patients, it is not known how leisure pursuits that would relate to reserve differ between patients and healthy controls. Such a comparison would be useful not only for understanding normative levels of reserve; they would also be helpful for elucidating how levels differ before and after illness. We thus sought to describe indicators of past and current reserve-building activities in a secondary analysis of a relatively large cohort of people with MS and healthy controls.

Methods

Sample

This secondary analysis utilized data from an ongoing prospective study of clinical, genetic and environmental risk factors in MS at the MS Center of the State University of New York at Buffalo which enrolled over 1,000 subjects with clinically isolated syndrome (CIS) [2, 17, 18], MS, healthy controls, and other neurologic diseases (OND) [19, 20]. The sample included 67 (8.3 %) people with CIS; 358 (44.2 %) people with relapsing-remitting MS (RRMS) and 109 people (13.5 %) with secondary progressive MS (SPMS). There were also 276 age- and sex-matched healthy controls. The inclusion criteria for this sub-analysis were presence of sufficient questionnaire data to obtain current and past reserve-building activities variables (i.e., the respondent was not missing data on the items assessing past and current reserve-building activities). The exclusion criteria were presence of relapse and steroid treatment in the 30 days preceding study entry for CIS and MS patients, pre-existing medical conditions known to be associated with brain pathology (cerebrovascular disease, positive history of alcohol abuse) and pregnancy. Healthy controls needed to meet the health-screen requirements, and had to have a normal physical and neurological examination. They were recruited from hospital personnel, or were respondents to a local advertisement. Table 1 provides demographic and clinical characteristics of the MS patient groupings and age-, sex- and race-matched healthy controls.

Table 1 Study participant demographics

Procedure

All subjects were assessed with a structured questionnaire administered in-person by a trained interviewer unaware of the subjects’ disease status [2]. This study was approved by the State University of New York at Buffalo Institutional Review Board (HSIRB #NEU2490109A) and written informed consent was obtained from all subjects.

Measures

The questionnaire contained information related to demographic characteristics, presence of autoimmune and other concomitant diseases, vascular risk factors and environmental factors, as well as information about habits. There were a set of questions addressing physically strenuous (i.e., exercise) activities as well as non-strenuous activities (e.g., hobbies or other pastimes). These questions reflected activities similar to those included in questionnaires investigating reserve-building activities [9, 13]. This analysis utilized items in the questionnaire containing information on activities relevant to building reserve and covered a subset of relevant past and current leisure activities. Based on the psychometric analyses described below, we created derived measures of reserve-building activities.

Statistical analysis

Data reduction

A series of data-reduction steps were used to generate composite scores for past and current reserve-building activity scores for analyses.

A past reserve-building activity score was created as educational and occupational attainment scores. An occupational attainment score was created using the O*NET OnLine Job Zones (ranging from one to five) based on how much education people need to do their work, how much related experience people need to do their work, and how much on-the-job training people need to do their work (http://www.onetonline.org/help/online/zones). When the job entry was not sufficiently specific to match only one O*NET job, the Job Zones for the relevant jobs were averaged. For example, a social worker may work with children, families, and schools (job zone 4), or with persons with mental health and substance abuse issues (job zone 5), giving them an occupational attainment score of 4.5. It should be noted that the focus of the O*NET coding was on level of occupational attainment of most recent/current job, not current employment status.

Current reserve-building activities were divided into strenuous and non-strenuous activities. Examples of strenuous activities included contact sports, aerobics, swimming, and wrestling. Example of non-strenuous activities included reading, browsing the internet, job related e-mail, meditation, and doing puzzles. Television-watching was also tracked in the data set and included in the analysis; it was considered a pastime unlikely to build reserve and which competed with reserve-building activities.

Survey questions addressing each activity were respectively analyzed using exploratory factor analysis (EFA) in Mplus statistical software [21]. An Mplus EFA was run on 18 strenuous items, leading to the eventual dropping items of that did not load above 0.40 on any of the factors (i.e., tai chi, other physical activity, and other aerobics). Based on the factor loadings and exercise-science conventions, we grouped the different strenuous activities into the following scores: Job related (job-related walking and moving); Organized Sports (contact sports, court sports, field sports); High Impact (aerobics, cross-training, running, weights); Low Impact (swimming, yoga, walking); Fighting Sports (wrestling, boxing); Computer/television-Related Exercise. An average score was calculated for each of the strenuous factors because the average was considered to be more interpretable than a sum. Each factor score was the average of the items it represented. A Strenuous Activity Summary Score was created by summing the strenuous factor scores.

An Mplus EFA with between one and nine factors was run on the 14 non-strenuous items using the ULS estimator and quartimin rotation. The output suggested a six- factor model based on the Kaiser criterion (Eigenvalues > 1). The quartimin rotated loadings were then used to group variables together, resulting in a reading factor (reading newspapers and reading magazines), television factor (television only), internet factor (browsing, social networking), job-related factor (job related reading, e-mails), spiritual factor (prayer, meditation), and game factor (cards, video games and puzzles). Each factor score was the average of the items it represented. A Non-Strenuous Activity Summary Score was created by summing the non-strenuous factor scores.

Multivariable analysis

To examine group differences in levels of past and current reserve-building activities, we tested three sets of multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) models with a Type I error rate of 5 %. MANOVA provides an omnibus test of statistical significance. If this omnibus test was statistically significant for the key independent variable (i.e., patient group), then post-hoc comparisons were run to identify which groups were statistically different from healthy controls. This statistical approach allows for control over the inflation of Type I error rate due to multiple comparisons [2]. A further strategy for adjusting for multiple comparisons was to prioritize the consistency of the four MANOVA output statistics (i.e., Wilk’s lambda, Lawley-Hotelling trace, Pillai’s trace, and Roy’s largest root). Group differences were interpreted when all four statistics were statistically significant [2]. A more conservative interpretation was used when between one and four of the MANOVA statistics was significant.

The dependent variables were: (a) past reserve-building activitiy scores; (b) strenuous current activity summary scores; (c) non-strenuous current activity summary scores. The independent variable was disease group (CIS, RRMS, and SPMS, with healthy controls as the comparison/referent group); age and gender were covariates. The MANOVA analyses of current Strenuous and Non-Strenuous activities also adjusted for education and occupational attainment to control for the effects of past reserve-building activities in explaining current reserve-building activities. Post-hoc estimation was done using multivariate regression. The MANOVA and regression analyses were implemented using Stata 13 [22].

Results

Past reserve-building activities

The MANOVA results suggested that there were possible group differences in past reserve-building activities, after adjusting for age and gender. The four MANOVA summary statistics were, however, not consistent (Table 2). Roy’s largest root was significant (i.e., p < 0.01) but Wilk’s lambda, Pillai’s trace, and Lawley-Hotelling trace were not significant. Post-hoc tests suggested that SPMS patients had lower occupational attainment and education level than healthy controls (p < 0.003 and 0.007, respectively).

Table 2 MANOVA test results

Current reserve-building activities

Strenuous activities

There were consistent group differences in strenuous current reserve-building pursuits (p < 0.0001), after adjusting for age, gender, education, and occupational attainment (Table 2; Fig. 1). Compared to healthy controls, people with all forms of MS engaged in fewer high-impact exercise (all p < 0.0001), low-impact exercise (all p < 0.0001), organized sports (all p < 0.0001), and job-related strenuous active pursuits (all p < 0.0001) (Table 3). RRMS and SPMS engaged in less computer- or television-based exercise (p < 0.02 and 0.05, respectively), and SPMS did fewer fighting sports (p < 0.05) (Table 3).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Strenuous activity scores by disease group. Healthy control scores are adjusted for age, gender, and the other disease groups are adjusted for age, gender, years since symptom onset, education, and occupational attainment

Table 3 Summary of Post-Hoc Comparisons

Non-strenuous activities

There were consistent group differences in non-strenuous reserve-building pursuits (p < 0.0001), after adjusting for age, gender, education, and occupational attainment (Tables 2 and 3; Fig. 2). RRMS read less than healthy controls (p < 0.01). SPMS engaged in less job-related reading and internet usage (p < 0.0001) and played more games (p < 0.05). CIS, RRMS, and SPMS watched more television than healthy controls (p < 0.02, 0.001, and 0.0001, respectively), but were similar in regard to internet usage, and spiritual pursuits.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Non-strenuous activity scores by disease group. Healthy control scores are adjusted for age, gender, and the other disease groups are adjusted for age, gender, years since symptom onset, education, and occupational attainment

Discussion

Although past research has documented a protective effect of current reserve-building activities on MS disability progression [5], our findings suggest that people with all forms of MS engaged in substantially fewer of the measured strenuous and non-strenuous current reserve-building pursuits than healthy controls. Further, they spent more time watching television, a leisure time activity that would not be considered stimulating or likely to maintain complex neural pathways. Since there is limited time in each day, time spent watching television necessarily takes away from available time for other leisure pursuits that may be health-enhancing. Accordingly, lifestyle-modifying interventions should prioritize replacing television-watching with more stimulating activities.

Our finding of a possible trend for people with progressive MS to report lower educational and occupational attainment has implications worth considering. First, it is possible that the illness itself may have limited achievement, since MS often strikes during young adulthood. Second, these putative differences may reflect that even education and occupation are relevant to current reserve-building activities; for example, it is possible that lower levels of past reserve-building activities pre-dispose individuals to lower levels of current reserve-building activities. Since a person builds his/her career over time, if s/he has MS, s/he will be more likely to be out of the work force earlier than expected. This may impact occupational attainment, as well as education level if continuing (i.e., graduate) education is at play. Again, these results should be interpreted with caution because the metric suggesting this statistical difference -- Roy’s largest root – may be more prone to significance than the other metrics [23].

Our findings have implications for understanding how MS affects people in ways beyond symptom experience, and suggest possible paths for intervention. While current reserve-building pursuits have been shown to have a protective effect on MS disease progression [5], our data suggest that MS patients participate in substantially fewer current strenuous reserve-building pursuits than healthy controls (Table 3). These findings are consistent with recent documentation that 80 % of people with MS do not meet recommended levels of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity [24, 25]. It is possible that early MS symptoms lead people to drop strenuous pursuits as they focus their energies on maintaining priority roles, such as employment and home-management activities. Motl recommends a paradigm shift in how exercise training is promoted within MS care, to focus on ‘lifestyle physical activity’ rather than ‘exercise training for fitness’ [26].

Our findings also suggest that other stimulating activities that are non-strenuous are also less prevalent among people with MS than among healthy controls. While some of these activities are linked to employment (i.e., job-related reading and email), others are not (e.g., reading, spiritual). Recent work done by members of our research team has documented that activities such as participating in a group or organization, traveling, visiting museums, attending lectures, participating in arts and crafts, cooking as a hobby, etc., are all associated with lower symptom burden cross-sectionally, and with slower disease progression over time. Even severely disabled people with MS were able to attain high scores on a reliable and content-valid measure of reserve, supporting the idea that these protective pursuits are accessible across the disability spectrum.

A logical next step in this line of research involves early intervention with people with MS. Focusing on promoting both strenuous and non-strenuous current reserve-building pursuits, such work would require helping patients to identify activities they would like to do across a range of domains, and helping them to mitigate perceived barriers and ensure regular practice of such pursuits. Similar to the paradigm shift proposed by Motl [26], such intervention work would need to help individuals expand their concept of salutogenic practice, and integrate such into their daily lives. An important first step might be to disable their television!

While our findings are interesting and generate hypotheses and intervention work for future research, the limitations of the present work must be acknowledged. First, as with any post-hoc study analysis, our data were not collected for our stated objectives; accordingly the operationalization of reserve was not as robust and multi-dimensional as it would be if we used the current validated measure of the construct [9, 27]. Since we did careful psychometric analysis of the items included in the reserve operationalization used in this study, we believe that it is a relatively valid proxy for reserve. Second, the cross-sectional nature of the data limits causal inference, such as these descriptive differences merely reflecting the disease itself. Past research done by members of our group has, however, suggested that even severely disabled MS patients can engage in current reserve-building pursuits that lead to a similar score as less disabled patients [5]. Nonetheless, future work might build on this study by comparing healthy controls and MS patient groups over time using the validated measure of reserve [9] to allow for causal inference.

Conclusions

In summary, our study documented that people with MS engage in fewer strenuous and non-strenuous current reserve-building pursuits, while watching more television than healthy controls. We believe the implications of these findings provide clear pathways for improving the health and well-being for people with MS, including lifestyle interventions that expand their repertoire of strenuous and non-strenuous reserve-builiding activities.

Abbreviations

CIS:

Clinically isolated syndrome

MS:

Multiple sclerosis

RRMS:

Relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis

SPMS:

Secondary progressive multiple sclerosis

References

  1. Ong AD, Bergeman CS, Bisconti TL, Wallace KA. Psychological resilience, positive emotions, and successful adaptation to stress in later life. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2006;91(4):730–49 [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't].

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Walsh R. Lifestyle and Mental Health. American Psychologist. 2011;66(7):579–92.

  3. Kuh D, Ben-Shlomo Y, Lynch J, Hallqvist J, Power C. Life course epidemiology. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003;57(10):778–83 [Review].

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve: Theory and applications. New York: Taylor & Francis; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Schwartz CE, Quaranto BR, Healy BC, Benedict RHB, Vollmer T. Cognitive reserve and symptom experience in multiple sclerosis: A buffer to disability progression over time? Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2013;94:1971–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Perneczky R, Alexopoulos P, Wagenpfeil S, Bickel H, Kurz A. Head circumference, apolipoprotein E genotype and cognition in the Bavarian School Sisters Study. Eur Psychiatry. 2012;27(3):219–22 [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't].

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Nithianantharajah J, Hannan AJ. The neurobiology of brain and cognitive reserve: mental and physical activity as modulators of brain disorders. Prog Neurobiol. 2009;89:36–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Sole-Padulles C, Bartres-Faz D, Junque C, Vendrell P, Rami L, Clemente IC, et al. Brain structure and function related to cognitive reserve variables in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30(7):1114–24.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Schwartz CE, Snook EM, Quaranto BR, Benedict RHB, Vollmer T. Cognitive reserve and patient-reported outcomes. MS Journal. 2013;19(1):87–105.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Schwartz CE, Quaranto BR, Healy BC, Benedict RH, Vollmer T. Altruism and health outcomes in multiple sclerosis: The effect of cognitive reserve. Journal of Positive Psychology. 2013;8(2):144–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Sumowski JF, Chiaravalloti N, DeLuca J. Cognitive reserve protects against cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2009;31(8):913–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Bigler ED. Traumatic brain injury and cognitive reserve. In: Stern Y, editor. Cognitive Reserve: Theory and Applications. New York: Taylor & Francis; 2007. p. 85–116.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Perneczky R, Drzezga A, Boecker H, Ceballos-Baumann AO, Granert O, Förstl H, et al. Activities of daily living, cerebral glucose metabolism, and cognitive reserve in Lewy body and Parkinson's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;26(5):475–81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Grady CL, editor. Cognitive reserve in healthy aging and Alzheimer disease: Evidence for compensatory reorganiztion of brain networks. New York: Taylor & Francis; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Ahles TA, Saykin AJ, McDonald BC, Li Y, Furstenberg CT, Hanscom BS, et al. Longitudinal assessment of cognitive changes associated with adjuvant treatment for breast cancer: Impact of age and cognitive reserve. J Clin Oncol. 2010;28(29):4434–40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Bleecker ML, Ford DP, Celio MA, Vaughan CG, Lindgren KN. Impact of cognitive reserve on the relationship of lead exposure and neurobehavioral performance. Neurology. 2007;69:470–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Cappellani R, Bergsland N, Weinstock-Guttman B, Kennedy C, Carl E, Ramasamy D, et al. Subcortical deep gray matter pathology in patients with multiple sclerosis is associated with white matter lesion burden and atrophy but not with cortical atrophy: a diffusion tensor MRI study. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2014;35(5):912–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Gabelic T, Ramasamy DP, Weinstock-Guttman B, Hagemeier J, Kennedy C, Melia R, et al. Prevalence of radiologically isolated syndrome and white matter signal abnormalities in healthy relatives of patients with multiple sclerosis. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2014;35(1):106–12.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. O'Connor K, Weinstock-Guttman B, Carl E, Kilanowski C, Zivadinov R, Ramanathan M. Patterns of dietary and herbal supplement use by multiple sclerosis patients. J Neurol. 2012;259(4):637–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Zivadinov R, Marr K, Cutter G, Ramanathan M, Benedict RH, Kennedy C, et al. Prevalence, sensitivity, and specificity of chronic cerebrospinal venous insufficiency in MS. Neurology. 2011;77(2):138–44.

  21. Muthèn LK, Muthèn BO. MPlus: Statistical analysis with latent variables version 5.2. Muthèn & Muthèn: Los Angeles, CA; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Stata 13. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP; 2013.

  23. Sciences NIoS. Roy's largest root VS all the other ones… . 2011 [07-10-2014]; Available from: http://www.talkstats.com/showthread.php/17207-Roy-s-largest-root-VS-all-the-other-ones

  24. Motl RW, McAuley E, Snook EM. Physical activity and multiple sclerosis: a meta-analysis. Mult Scler. 2005;11(4):459–63 [Comparative Study, Meta-Analysis, Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't].

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Klaren RE, Motl RW, Dlugonski D, Sandroff BM, Pilutti LA. Objectively quantified physical activity in persons with multiple sclerosis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2013;94(12):2342–8 [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural].

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Motl RW. Lifestyle physical activity in persons with multiple sclerosis: the new kid on the MS block. Mult Scler. 2014;20(8):1025–9 [Review].

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Schwartz CE, Ayandeh A, Rodgers J, Duberstein P, Weinstock-Guttman B, Benedict RBB, editors. A new perspective on proxy report: Implicit processes of understanding and reserve. Quality of Life Research. 2015, online first. doi: 10.1007/S11136-015-1017-4.

Download references

Acknowledgements

Funding for this work was provided by the Buffalo Neuroimaging Analysis Center research fund.

We are grateful for advice from Rita Bode, Ph.D., in early analyses related to this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Carolyn E. Schwartz.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

CES planned and carried out the analysis, and drafted the manuscript. AA performed the statistical analysis, and helped to draft the manuscript. MR, RB, MGD, BWG, and RZ participated in the design of the study, and helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ information

CES is President and Chief Scientist at DeltaQuest Foundation, a not-for-profit organization in Concord, MA; Adjunct Research Professor of Medicine and Orthopaedic Surgery at Tufts University Medical School in Boston, MA; Adjunct Professor of Nursing at the Oslo and Akershus University College in Oslo, Norway. A behavioral scientist with expertise in quality of life, patient-reported measurement development, psychometric and statistical analysis, and multiple sclerosis clinical research, she is also the Co-Editor-in-Chief of Quality of Life Research since 2009. RZ is Professor of Neurology at the School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, State University of New York, Buffalo, NY, USA. He has expertise in imaging and epidemiology of multiple sclerosis clinical research.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Schwartz, C.E., Ayandeh, A., Ramanathan, M. et al. Reserve-building activities in multiple sclerosis patients and healthy controls: a descriptive study. BMC Neurol 15, 135 (2015). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12883-015-0395-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12883-015-0395-0

Keywords