Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Cognition, behaviour and academic skills after cognitive rehabilitation in Ugandan children surviving severe malaria: a randomised trial

Abstract

Background

Infection with severe malaria in African children is associated with not only a high mortality but also a high risk of cognitive deficits. There is evidence that interventions done a few years after the illness are effective but nothing is known about those done immediately after the illness. We designed a study in which children who had suffered from severe malaria three months earlier were enrolled into a cognitive intervention program and assessed for the immediate benefit in cognitive, academic and behavioral outcomes.

Methods

This parallel group randomised study was carried out in Kampala City, Uganda between February 2008 and October 2010. Sixty-one Ugandan children aged 5 to 12 years with severe malaria were assessed for cognition (using the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, second edition and the Test of Variables of Attention), academic skills (Wide Range Achievement Test, third edition) and psychopathologic behaviour (Child Behaviour Checklist) three months after an episode of severe malaria. Twenty-eight were randomised to sixteen sessions of computerised cognitive rehabilitation training lasting eight weeks and 33 to a non-treatment group. Post-intervention assessments were done a month after conclusion of the intervention. Analysis of covariance was used to detect any differences between the two groups after post-intervention assessment, adjusting for age, sex, weight for age z score, quality of the home environment, time between admission and post-intervention testing and pre-intervention score. The primary outcome was improvement in attention scores for the intervention group. This trial is registered with Current Controlled Trials, number ISRCTN53183087.

Results

Significant intervention effects were observed in the intervention group for learning mean score (SE), [93.89 (4.00) vs 106.38 (4.32), P = 0.04] but for working memory the intervention group performed poorly [27.42 (0.66) vs 25.34 (0.73), P = 0.04]. No effect was observed in the other cognitive outcomes or in any of the academic or behavioural measures.

Conclusions

In this pilot study, our computerised cognitive training program three months after severe malaria had an immediate effect on cognitive outcomes but did not affect academic skills or behaviour. Larger trials with follow-up after a few years are needed to investigate whether the observed benefits are sustained.

Trial registration

ISRCTN: ISRCTN53183087

Peer Review reports

Background

Cognitive deficits are common in children who have suffered from severe malaria having neurological involvement [1]. Severe malaria with neurological involvement (MNI) manifests as; deep coma (i.e. cerebral malaria), seizures, impaired consciousness, prostration and agitated behaviour [2, 3]. Commonly affected cognitive functions are memory, attention, visual spatial ability, learning and language [4–10]. With cognitive impairment rates of between 14 to 26%, it is estimated that over 200,000 children in sub-Saharan Africa are left with impaired cognition annually after an episode of MNI [6, 9]. Moreover, the occurrence of these cognitive impairments and their severity appears to be more evident two years later [9] suggesting that cognitive impairment becomes more apparent after a few years as children are required to perform more complex cognitive tasks. Psychopathologic behaviour are also increasingly being recognised in children who have suffered from MNI with hyperactive behaviours, conduct problems, autism like behaviours and other behavioural problems being reported in recent studies [11, 12].

Malaria is thus a leading cause of neuro-disability in sub-Saharan Africa [1] and better interventions for child survivors are necessary [13]. Some of the suggested interventions include computerised cognitive rehabilitation training, caregiver training, early child education, nutritional support, physical therapy and speech therapy [13]. Computerised cognitive rehabilitation training (CCRT) may offer the best solution as it can be programmed to target specific functions, has proven effective in different forms of brain injury in both children and adults [14–20] and brings about physiological changes in the brain like increased brain activity, dopamine availability and changes in white matter structure [21–24]. Increased brain activity, availability of dopamine in the prefrontal areas and white matter integrity are in turn associated with performance of cognitive functions like working memory and executive functions [25–28].

In one of the pioneering applications of CCRT in sub-Saharan, children who suffered from cerebral malaria approximately four years earlier were randomised to either CCRT or a non-treatment group [29]. Post-intervention assessments showed improved memory, learning, psychomotor speed and internalising behaviour in the intervention group. This study however had some limitations. Firstly it was carried out almost four years after the illness yet studies indicate that cognitive test performance is poorer two years after the illness compared to controls [9]. This may probably explain why there was no improvement in visual attention yet impaired attention was the only cognitive deficit observed in these children two years earlier [9]. It is thus possible that interventions done some years after the illness may have little benefit since the cognitive impact of the disease is more apparent much later. Early interventions may stall the development of these deficits. Secondly, a computerised test of cognition was used and given that the intervention was also computerised, the observed benefits may have been a consequence of the familiarity with the computer.

We present a study addressing the above limitations whose objective was to evaluate the effect of CCRT given immediately after recovery from severe malaria (at three months) on cognition, behaviour and academic skills. Inclusion of academic skills will provide more information to an ongoing debate whether CCRT improves performance in other functional skills that are not directly trained by the intervention [30, 31].

Methods

Study population and recruitment

Children were recruited from Mulago Hospital, the National Referral Hospital of Uganda, and from Nsambya, Rubaga and Mengo Hospitals all located in Kampala, Uganda's Capital City. Recruitment and follow up was done between February 2008 and October 2010. The latter three are large private mission hospitals. Participants were children aged 5 to 12 years presenting with a fever and Plasmodium falciparum on blood smears and one or more of the following; 1) a seizure lasting over 15 minutes or repeated seizures, 2) impaired consciousness (Glasgow coma scale score of 14 and below), 3) unarousable coma with normal cerebrospinal fluid findings. Children meeting these three criteria were categorised as malaria with seizures, malaria with impaired consciousness or cerebral malaria respectively. A physical examination and medical history were done on admission. The exclusion criteria were: history of or present meningitis or encephalitis, prior central nervous system infection, sickle cell disease, epilepsy, history of developmental delay and history of hospitalization for malnutrition.

Ninety children were recruited for the study. Of these, 10 did not meet the above inclusion criteria and were excluded and one died during admission leaving 79 children for follow-up. At discharge, home directions and telephone contacts were obtained from the parents/caregivers and appointment dates given for the baseline assessment three months later. In the interim period, a home visit was made to assess the quality of the home environment. By the three months hospital visit, 18 children had either withdrawn from the study or had been lost to follow-up or were neurologically impaired to participate in the study leaving 61 children who were given assessments of cognition, behaviour and academic skills. At the completion of these assessments, stratified randomisation for the three MNI groups (cerebral malaria, malaria with seizures and malaria with impaired consciousness) to either the CCRT group (N = 28) or the non-treatment group (N = 33) was done (see figure 1). A table of random numbers was used for group allocation. Post-intervention assessment was done a month after the intervention training. Based on our earlier study where the mean difference in attention between the two groups was 0.18 with a standard deviation of 0.24 [9], a study with a power of 80% would require 30 children in each arm. The above sample size gives a power of 78%.

Figure 1
figure 1

Flow of participants in the study.

Written informed consent was obtained from the parents or guardians of study participants and assent from children aged seven years and older. Ethical approval for this study was granted by the Institutional Review Board for Human Studies at Makerere University College of Health Sciences and the Uganda National Council for Science and Technology.

Intervention

The CCRT package used was Captain's Log software [32] consisting of 35 multi-level brain-training exercises designed to help develop and remediate a wide range of cognitive skills. Fifteen of the 35 possible brain-training exercises were chosen for this study. The criteria for deciding which exercises to include were: (1) having little or no verbal instructions so that children with poor grasp of English would benefit and (2) having simple or few movements with the track-ball. Pretesting demonstrated that Ugandan children who were, for the most part unfamiliar with computers, would be more comfortable using a track-ball than a mouse, particularly if required movements were not large.

Four exercises were chosen to train attention; (1) scanning reaction/inhibition (clicking the mouse if the color of several varying images matches the color of the screen's border), (2) stimulus reaction time (the player is required to click the mouse once if the "target" image appears), (3) stimulus reaction/fields (the player is required to move the mouse and click it over the "target" image), and (4) stimulus reaction/inhibition (clicking the mouse if the color of several random images appearing one at a time matches the color of the screen's border).

The memory training exercises were; (5) conceptual (finding the missing part of a sequence from several choices), (6) logical sequences (finding and clicking on targets in the correct sequence), (7) size discrimination (clicking on target objects in order according to size), and (8) symbolic display match (selecting and placing targets in the correct box based on various rules).

Visuomotor Skills was trained by three exercises; (9) visual categorization (clicking on object that appears from behind a door according to the category rule); (10) visual response time (watching a grid of targets and clicking on any that changes), (11) visuospatial memory (searching for and find matching objects in a grid).

The four exercises for reasoning were; (12) concept logic (figuring out the secret rule in a number of images), (13) match logic (deciding whether images match or not), (14) picture logic (clicking on the target among foils), and (15) sequential logic (understanding the conceptual rules in respect to the logic of number/letter patterns).

Four exercises were devoted to attention training than other cognitive abilities since it is a commonly observed deficit after severe malaria. Captain's Log was programmed to run for 45 minutes with the first training session starting at the simplest level and the difficulty increased based on the child's performance. Children performed two sessions once a week for eight weeks.

Assessments

Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children second edition (KABC-II)

The KABC-II is a comprehensive assessment of cognitive ability containing a number of scales that have been adapted, piloted and validated among African children in Kenya [4], Senegal [5] and Uganda [8]. It retains its construct validity when used in Ugandan children to access Working Memory (taking in and holding information and then using it within a few seconds), Visual Spatial ability (perceiving, storing, manipulating and thinking with visual patterns), Learning (storing and efficiently retrieving newly learned or previously learned information) and Planning (solving novel problems by using reasoning abilities like induction and deduction) [33].

Test of Variables of Attention (TOVA)

The TOVA is a computer administered visual continuous performance test used in the diagnosis and monitoring of children and adults with attention deficit disorders [34]. The TOVA has been used in previous malaria studies in Senegal and Uganda [5, 8, 9]. It is a sensitive measure of cerebral insult from malaria as indicated by the persisting attention deficits at 24 months in Ugandan children with cerebral malaria [9]. The outcome measure was the d' prime score, which is the child's accuracy in discriminating between the target and non-target stimuli.

Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL)

The CBCL is a paper-pencil child behavioural rating scale consisting of 120 items to which a parent/guardian responds [35]. It has been validated in 30 societies, has proven useful in multicultural assessment of children [36] and has fair reliability in Ugandan children [12]. Studies examining the structure of psychopathology have identified internalising and externalising symptoms as the two broad factors underlying psychopathology [37, 38]. The externalising factor is comprised of substance dependence, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, and conduct disorder/antisocial personality disorder. The internalising factor has 'distress/misery' comprising generalised anxiety disorder, overanxious disorder and depressive disorders and 'fear', which includes simple and social phobias, separation anxiety disorder and panic disorder [37]. These two broad categories of internalising and externalising psychopathologic behaviour are measured by the CBCL and were the outcomes for behaviour in this study.

Wide Range Achievement Test-third edition (WRAT-3)

The WRAT-3 is a measure of the codes needed to learn the basic skills of reading, spelling and arithmetic [39, 40]. It has been used earlier for research in Ugandan children with HIV [41].

Middle Childhood Home Observations for the Measurement of the Environment (MC-HOME)

The MC-HOME [42] identifies parental behaviours that are important to children's cognitive development and academic skills. The MC-HOME as adapted to the Ugandan setting by Boivin and colleagues [8] was used in this study. It has 58 items measuring the amount of stimulation and learning opportunities available to the child in the home, summed into a single score. The MC-HOME measures the quality of the home environment that is predictive of working memory performance in Ugandan children [43].

Since attention was shown as the persisting deficit at 24 months in Ugandan children [9], attention capacity measured by the TOVA was the primary outcome of this study. Attention deficits in severe malaria may affect other cognitive skills that are dependent on it which involve vigilance and perceptual acuity [5]. These other cognitive skills were the secondary outcomes for cognition; working memory, visual spatial ability, reasoning and learning. For academic skills, reading, spelling and arithmetic were the outcomes and for behaviour, they were internalising and externalising problems.

Data analysis

Data was analysed using SPSS 17.0. Log transformation was done where appropriate for variables that were not normally distributed prior to statistical analysis. To assess the effect of the intervention while controlling for other covariates, analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was run on the post-intervention score with covariates age, weight for age z scores, quality of the home environment, sex, time between admission and post-intervention assessment and baseline pre-intervention score on the same outcome variable. Data are presented as estimated means (with standard errors) between the intervention and control groups.

Results

Children in the treatment and non-treatment groups had similar demographic characteristics as shown in table 1. Importantly, there was a similar distribution of children with cerebral malaria, malaria with seizures and malaria with impaired consciousness between the treatment and control groups. The mean period between the date of admission and the post intervention testing was approximately seven months (range 5.43 to 10.47 months). At baseline, both groups had similar cognitive, academic and behavioural scores (Table 2). In addition, there were no differences in the outcomes scores between the three different malaria groups (data not shown).

Table 1 Participants' demographic characteristics
Table 2 Baseline scores1 between the two groups

Intervention effects were observed in learning mean score (SE) [93.89 (4.00) vs 106.38 (4.32), P = 0.04]. For working memory however, the effect showed improvement in the control group and not the treatment group [27.42 (0.66) vs 25.34 (0.73), P = 0.04. No intervention effects were observed in attention, the primary outcome or in any of the other cognitive outcomes or in academic skills and behavioural measures as shown in Table 3.

Table 3 Mean scores post-intervention between intervention and control groups

There were interaction effects between treatment group and sex for Arithmetic only with the boys in the intervention group having a better score than those in the control arm (mean difference in adjusted scores between intervention and control group (standard error) 1.13 (0.81): P = 0.17). For the girls, those in the intervention group had poorer scores than those in the control arm (-3.63 (1.94): P = 0.08).

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate the effect of CCRT on cognition, academic skills and behaviour in children after an episode of severe malaria. Analyses controlling for possible confounders found an intervention benefit in learning.

Our present findings of some benefit on cognition from CCRT are similar to recent findings from Uganda done among children with cerebral malaria and HIV showing improvement in some cognitive domains after the intervention [19, 29]. Both these studies showed an improvement in learning as we did. The current study however showed a positive benefit in only one outcome out of the ten tested. In addition, the intervention group had poorer working memory scores after the intervention. In some of the recent successful CCRT studies with children, more training sessions were done than the 16 sessions in our study, which may explain why minimal effect was seen as the intervention was not intense [15, 16, 20, 44].

Improvement in some outcomes may however not be evident if assessed within a month after the intervention as we did. Holmes at al did not notice any benefit in academic skills a week after cognitive training in children but noticed a benefit at six months instead [44]. They concluded that any cognitive support to learning caused by training would be expected to take some time to work before any benefits are observed.

In other studies, CCRT has been shown to improve cognitive functioning in children with different forms of brain injury, including cerebral malaria, HIV, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and brain tumours [16, 17, 19, 20, 29]. Some of these studies showed benefits at three and six months follow up [16, 20]. We can't readily state whether our intervention's benefits will be sustained in these children with malaria since we have not done follow-up evaluations. Improvements in cognition after training have been associated with physiological changes in the brain that are linked with cognitive development [21–24]. In addition, cognitive training has improved other skills that are not trained like academic scores and everyday behaviour [16, 17, 29, 44]. We did not see differences in academic scores or behaviour scores in our study, possibly because the selected cognitive exercises in our program are not effective for these outcomes.

It is not yet clear whether CCRT at three months or at four years for severe malaria survivors will produce better results. In our two studies with severe malaria survivors, CCRT at four years had better results than the current study with an earlier intervention. However, since all the post-intervention assessments were immediate, we cannot draw firm conclusions from these studies about optimal timing of interventions for sustained benefit.

A major limitation of this study was the design of the intervention program. Whereas our cognitive intervention studies have used the same cognitive regimen training several abilities, other studies have instead trained only working memory but still shown benefits in other cognitive skills [16, 44]. Training a specific cognitive ability might produce better results than what we have observed in our studies because more time is given to training a specific individual ability. In addition, accomplishing 16 training sessions was intensive in our study area, but this number of sessions is fewer than what some other effective studies have used [15, 16, 20, 44]. It is therefore important to establish what skills to train and how many sessions are adequate for a detectable benefit to be observed.

Other limitations of this study include; no follow up several months later, no active control group, a small sample size and lack of blinding of assessors to the children's treatment group. A larger trial with a careful study design is currently being set up to address some of the above limitations.

Conclusions

We conclude that our cognitive intervention targeting different cognitive abilities may improve some cognitive abilities but not academic skills and behaviour three months after severe malaria. Future studies need to assess whether the effects of computerised cognitive rehabilitation training in children with severe malaria are sustained over time.

References

  1. Idro R, Marsh K, John CC, Newton CR: Cerebral malaria: mechanisms of brain injury and strategies for improved neurocognitive outcome. Pediatr Res. 2010, 68 (4): 267-274. 10.1203/PDR.0b013e3181eee738.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Idro R, Ndiritu M, Ogutu B, Mithwani S, Maitland K, Berkley J, Crawley J, Fegan G, Bauni E, Peshu N, Marsh K, Neville B, Newton C: Burden, features, and outcome of neurological involvement in acute falciparum malaria in Kenyan children. JAMA. 2007, 297 (20): 2232-2240. 10.1001/jama.297.20.2232.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Newton CR, Warrell DA: Neurological manifestations of falciparum malaria. Ann Neurol. 1998, 43 (6): 695-702. 10.1002/ana.410430603.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Holding PA, Stevenson J, Peshu N, Marsh K: Cognitive sequelae of severe malaria with impaired consciousness. Trans R Soc Trop Med Hyg. 1999, 93 (5): 529-534. 10.1016/S0035-9203(99)90368-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Boivin MJ: Effects of early cerebral malaria on cognitive ability in Senegalese children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2002, 23 (5): 353-364. 10.1097/00004703-200210000-00010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Carter JA, Ross AJ, Neville BG, Obiero E, Katana K, Mung'ala-Odera V, Lees JA, Newton CR: Developmental impairments following severe falciparum malaria in children. Trop Med Int Health. 2005, 10 (1): 3-10. 10.1111/j.1365-3156.2004.01345.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Kihara M, Carter JA, Newton CR: The effect of Plasmodium falciparum on cognition: a systematic review. Trop Med Int Health. 2006, 11 (4): 386-397. 10.1111/j.1365-3156.2006.01579.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Boivin MJ, Bangirana P, Byarugaba J, Opoka RO, Idro R, Jurek AM, John CC: Cognitive impairment after cerebral malaria in children: a prospective study. Pediatrics. 2007, 119 (2): e360-366. 10.1542/peds.2006-2027.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. John CC, Bangirana P, Byarugaba J, Opoka RO, Idro R, Jurek AM, Wu B, Boivin MJ: Cerebral malaria in children is associated with long-term cognitive impairment. Pediatrics. 2008, 122 (1): e92-99. 10.1542/peds.2007-3709.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Carter JA, Lees JA, Gona JK, Murira G, Rimba K, Neville BG, Newton CR: Severe falciparum malaria and acquired childhood language disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006, 48 (1): 51-57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Idro R, Kakooza-Mwesige A, Balyejjussa S, Mirembe G, Mugasha C, Tugumisirize J, Byarugaba J: Severe neurological sequelae and behaviour problems after cerebral malaria in Ugandan children. BMC Res Notes. 2010, 3: 104-10.1186/1756-0500-3-104.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Bangirana P, Nakasujja N, Giordani B, Opoka RO, John CC, Boivin MJ: Reliability of the Luganda version of the Child Behaviour Checklist in measuring behavioural problems after cerebral malaria. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2009, 3: 38-10.1186/1753-2000-3-38.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Bangirana P, Idro R, John CC, Boivin MJ: Rehabilitation for cognitive impairments after cerebral malaria in African children: strategies and limitations. Trop Med Int Health. 2006, 11 (9): 1341-1349. 10.1111/j.1365-3156.2006.01685.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Westerberg H, Jacobaeus H, Hirvikoski T, Clevberger P, Ostensson ML, Bartfai A, Klingberg T: Computerized working memory training after stroke--a pilot study. Brain Inj. 2007, 21 (1): 21-29. 10.1080/02699050601148726.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Sjo NM, Spellerberg S, Weidner S, Kihlgren M: Training of attention and memory deficits in children with acquired brain injury. Acta Paediatr. 2010, 99 (2): 230-236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Klingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen PJ, Johnson M, Gustafsson P, Dahlstrom K, Gillberg CG, Forssberg H, Westerberg H: Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD--a randomized, controlled trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005, 44 (2): 177-186. 10.1097/00004583-200502000-00010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Shalev L, Tsal Y, Mevorach C: Computerized progressive attentional training (CPAT) program: effective direct intervention for children with ADHD. Child Neuropsychol. 2007, 13 (4): 382-388. 10.1080/09297040600770787.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Poletti S, Anselmetti S, Bechi M, Ermoli E, Bosia M, Smeraldi E, Cavallaro R: Computer-aided neurocognitive remediation in schizophrenia: durability of rehabilitation outcomes in a follow-up study. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2010, 20 (5): 659-674. 10.1080/09602011003683158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Boivin MJ, Busman RA, Parikh SM, Bangirana P, Page CF, Opoka RO, Giordani B: A pilot study of the neuropsychological benefits of computerized cognitive rehabilitation in Ugandan children with HIV. Neuropsychology. 2010, 24 (5): 667-673.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. van 't Hooft I, Andersson K, Bergman B, Sejersen T, von Wendt L, Bartfai A: Sustained favorable effects of cognitive training in children with acquired brain injuries. NeuroRehabilitation. 2007, 22 (2): 109-116.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Olesen PJ, Westerberg H, Klingberg T: Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory. Nat Neurosci. 2004, 7 (1): 75-79. 10.1038/nn1165.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. McNab F, Varrone A, Farde L, Jucaite A, Bystritsky P, Forssberg H, Klingberg T: Changes in cortical dopamine D1 receptor binding associated with cognitive training. Science. 2009, 323 (5915): 800-802. 10.1126/science.1166102.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Takeuchi H, Sekiguchi A, Taki Y, Yokoyama S, Yomogida Y, Komuro N, Yamanouchi T, Suzuki S, Kawashima R: Training of Working Memory Impacts Structural Connectivity. J Neurosci. 2010, 30 (9): 3297-3303. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4611-09.2010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Hoekzema E, Carmona S, Tremols V, Gispert JD, Guitart M, Fauquet J, Rovira M, Bielsa A, Soliva JC, Tomas X, Bulbena A, Ramos A, Casas M, Tobena A, Vilarroya O: Enhanced neural activity in frontal and cerebellar circuits after cognitive training in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010, 31 (12): 1942-1950. 10.1002/hbm.20988.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Aalto S, Bruck A, Laine M, Nagren K, Rinne JO: Frontal and Temporal Dopamine Release during Working Memory and Attention Tasks in Healthy Humans: a Positron Emission Tomography Study Using the High-Affinity Dopamine D2 Receptor Ligand [11C]FLB 457. J Neurosci. 2005, 25 (10): 2471-2477. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2097-04.2005.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Klingberg T, Forssberg H, Westerberg H: Increased brain activity in frontal and parietal cortex underlies the development of visuospatial working memory capacity during childhood. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002, 14 (1): 1-10. 10.1162/089892902317205276.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Klingberg T: Development of a superior frontal-intraparietal network for visuo-spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia. 2006, 44 (11): 2171-2177. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.11.019.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Ko JH, Ptito A, Monchi O, Cho SS, Van Eimeren T, Pellecchia G, Ballanger B, Rusjan P, Houle S, Strafella AP: Increased dopamine release in the right anterior cingulate cortex during the performance of a sorting task: A [11C]FLB 457 PET study. Neuroimage. 2009, 46 (2): 516-521. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.031.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Bangirana P, Giordani B, John CC, Page C, Opoka RO, Boivin MJ: Immediate Neuropsychological and Behavioral Benefits of Computerized Cognitive Rehabilitation in Ugandan Pediatric Cerebral Malaria Survivors. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2009, 30 (4): 310-318. 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181b0f01b.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Owen AM, Hampshire A, Grahn JA, Stenton R, Dajani S, Burns AS, Howard RJ, Ballard CG: Putting brain training to the test. Nature. 2010, 465 (7299): 775-778. 10.1038/nature09042.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Dickinson D, Tenhula W, Morris S, Brown C, Peer J, Spencer K, Li L, Gold JM, Bellack AS: A randomized, controlled trial of computer-assisted cognitive remediation for schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2010, 167 (2): 170-180. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09020264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Sandford JA: Captain's Log Computerized Cognitive Training System. 2007, Richmond, VA: Brain Train

    Google Scholar 

  33. Bangirana P, Musisi S, Allebeck P, Giordani B, John CC, Opoka OR, Byarugaba J, Ehnvall A, Boivin MJ: A preliminary examination of the construct validity of the KABC-II in Ugandan children with a history of cerebral malaria. African Health Sciences. 2009, 9 (3): 188-192.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Dupuy TR, Greenberg LM: The T.O.V.A. Manual for IBM Personal Computer or IBM Compatible. 2005, Minneapolis, MN: Universal Attention Disorders

    Google Scholar 

  35. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA: Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles: an integrated system of multi-informant assessment. 2001, Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families

    Google Scholar 

  36. Ivanova M, Achenbach T, Dumenci L, Rescorla L, Almqvist F, Weintraub S, Bilenberg N, Bird H, Chen W, Dobrean A, et al: Testing the 8-Syndrome Structure of the Child Behavior Checklist in 30 Societies. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2007, 36 (3): 405-417. 10.1080/15374410701444363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Clark LA, Watson D: Distress and fear disorders: an alternative empirically based taxonomy of the 'mood' and 'anxiety' disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2006, 189: 481-483. 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.03825.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Slade T, Watson D: The structure of common DSM-IV and ICD-10 mental disorders in the Australian general population. Psychological Medicine. 2006, 36: 1593-1600. 10.1017/S0033291706008452.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Wilkinson GS: Wide-Range Achievement Test 3: Administration Manual. 1993, Wilmington, Del.: Wide Range

    Google Scholar 

  40. Knoop A: Test Review. Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin. 2004, 47 (3): 184-185. 10.1177/00343552040470030801.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Bagenda D, Nassali A, Kalyesubula I, Sherman B, Drotar D, Boivin MJ, Olness K: Health, neurologic, and cognitive status of HIV-infected, long-surviving, and antiretroviral-naive Ugandan children. Pediatrics. 2006, 117 (3): 729-740. 10.1542/peds.2004-2699.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Caldwell BM, Bradley RH: Home Inventory Administration Manual. 2001, Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas, 3

    Google Scholar 

  43. Bangirana P, John CC, Idro R, Opoka RO, Byarugaba J, Jurek AM, Boivin MJ: Socioeconomic predictors of cognition in Ugandan children: implications for community interventions. PLoS One. 2009, 4 (11): e7898-10.1371/journal.pone.0007898.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Holmes J, Gathercole SE, Dunning DL: Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Dev Sci. 2009, 12 (4): F9-15. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00848.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank the parents and children that took part in this study and the different members of the research team from the Makerere University-University of Minnesota Cerebral Malaria Project, Acute Care Unit in Mulago Hospital and Nsambya, Rubaga and Mengo hospitals for their contribution to this study. Assessment materials for the Test of Variables of Attention were provided by Dr. Robert Leark on behalf of the T.O.V.A Research Foundation and Universal Attention Disorders, Inc., 4281 Katella #215, Los Alamitos, California, USA.

Funding

This study was funded in part by grants from The Swedish Institute to Paul Bangirana for the Joint Makerere University/Karolinska Institutet PhD Program; Michigan State University Department of Neurology faculty start-up funding to Michael J Boivin and National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke award (5R01NS055349) to Chandy C. John.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul Bangirana.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

PB conceived the study, participated in the data collection, analysis, interpretation and writing of the manuscript. PA, MJB, CCJ, AE and SM participated in the interpretation and writing of the manuscript. CP participated in the, analysis, interpretation and writing of the manuscript. All authors read and gave approval of the final version to be published.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bangirana, P., Allebeck, P., Boivin, M.J. et al. Cognition, behaviour and academic skills after cognitive rehabilitation in Ugandan children surviving severe malaria: a randomised trial. BMC Neurol 11, 96 (2011). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-2377-11-96

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-2377-11-96

Keywords